To help us learn about the components and processes
involved in an information processing model, we will refer to the
flow diagram below.

This diagram has been developed from the original model proposed
by Atkinson and Shiffrin in 1968. In the diagram, we can follow
the flow of information through the various stages. We will then
look at each of the stages in a little more detail.
In the diagram, you need to trace the flow of
information from the inputs on the left, the ‘Environmental
stimuli’, following the arrows to the Sensory Register (SR),
then through the processes of ‘attention’ and ‘perception’
to the Short-term (STM) and Working memory (WM). From that point
an arrow labeled ‘encoding’ continues to the box on
the right, the Long-term memory (LTM). The arrow ‘retrieval’
then returns to the WM, and a ‘response’ is created
(the output). You will notice that in following the process, information
is lost both from the SR and the STM – this is because every
day we are confronted with far too much information to process,
so it is necessary to pay attention to some and to allow a great
deal not to be processed (otherwise we would be overwhelmed with
all the demands on our minds). In the diagram, you will also notice
that 3 boxes are labelled ‘Information-processing components’
– these are the SR, STM/WM and LTM . These are important basic
structures and information is stored and moves between these. The
cognitive processes in which we are interested are: attention, perception,
encoding and retrieval.
We will now describe each of the stages in more
detail. An important note for you here is this: although we break
down and describe these components and processes step-by-step, they
occur in a split-second (very quickly), without us being conscious
of them. We hope that as you learn about these, you will become
more aware of how you process information, as you watch what comes
into your sensory store, what you pay attention to, the things you
decide you want to remember, the way in which you rehearse things
to assist in memorizing, and how you think about retrieving information
from your memory.
Environmental stimuli: These
are all the things we notice in our surroundings, using our senses.
We see, hear, taste, touch and smell a great deal each day. In order
for a stimulus to move from our senses to the SR, we need to pay
attention to that stimulus – and thus our internal mental
process begins.
Sensory register (SR): The information
from the environment is stored very briefly – often for less
than half a second in our SR. What we pay attention to is then important
in what happens next. If our working memory is relatively free of
work, we might pay attention to the new input. But, if we are concentrating
on something else, we don’t notice or quickly forget the new
incoming information. (You know this experience – if you are
reading something very interesting, although you may know the radio
is playing in the next room, you will not ‘hear’ what
is played). If the incoming information is of importance to you,
you might shift your attention to another stimulus – for example
when your sister shouts to you “Supper is ready”. A
related process to attention – perception – also plays
a role here. We perceive information when we recognize something
familiar happening in the environment, or we recognize a pattern
of letters, sounds, and so on. Perception is the way in which we
try to find meaning in a stimulus.
For example, look at the symbols on the next line:
- ) :
The symbols are a dash, a bracket and a colon.
But what do you see if we arrange them as follows?
: - )
Someone busy and not interested might simply perceive
the same symbols arranged in a different way. If you were interested
in the symbols or had experience of working on a computer you may
have tried to find meaning in the stimulus and perceived a happy
face, two eyes, a nose and a smiling mouth. Some computers automatically
change the symbols :-) into a happy face
Short-term memory (STM): Paying
attention to or perceiving the meaning of a stimulus transfers it
to the short-term memory or working memory. Information is stored
here for a few seconds – for example if you’ve looked
up a telephone number in the book, you store it here so you can
dial the number, but then forget the number after a little while.
The working memory is like a note pad where we note ideas and work
with them, maybe changing their order – so we keep going over
the information in different ways in WM to help us think about it.
If we don’t process or work with something in STM we quickly
lose it. The speed of forgetting is shown in the following graph
(Figure 1.3).

Figure 1.3 Decay of
information in short term memory
Here we see the performance of learners who learned
meaningless sets of three letters called trigrams (e.g. YGM, XJR).
When they had to immediately recall these, the average subject remembered
88% of them. After 3 seconds (during which time subjects had to
count numbers), they remembered only 62%. After an 18 second interval,
only 15% could be remembered. This illustrates how quickly information
is lost from STM. In self-activity 1.5 there are some memory tasks
for you to try.
Long-term memory (LTM): What
can we do to assist ourselves and our learners to store important
information?
In order for information to be transferred to
LTM, it needs to be encoded – this means that the information
is linked to something we already know, or is re-arranged to make
it meaningful. (This is where what we know about schema’s,
assimilation and accommodation is helpful). Often it is ‘coded’
by us identifying only ‘key’ or important words that
prompt the associated thoughts. In order to store the information
for a much longer time, we often need to rehearse, or ‘go
over’ the information a number of times in our WM. The LTM
seems to have a large capacity, and the information decays over
a long time only if we do not access it, or forget how to retrieve
it (that is, forget the ‘code’ for remembering it).
Retrieval is being able to access the information we have stored
in our LTM, so that we can work with it in the WM, or give a response.
The decay of memory in the LTM is often related to how meaningful
the information is for the learner. If the material has little meaning
or is not understood, it decays quickly, often within minutes. If
it is linked to facts, it will last a little longer, but if the
learner has no interest in the facts, it will be lost. When learners
are interested in material, and work with it so that they can describe
it in their own words, and revise the material at various intervals
in the course, they are much more likely to be able to access it
from their LTM.
|