
Figure 1
The two drawings in Figure 1 illustrate the change
in a boy’s drawings of himself between ages 6 and 11. When
we look at the way in which children’s drawings change from
an early scribble to the intricate drawings of the adolescent, we
are seeing an expression of the many cognitive changes which children
experience as they mature.
Self-Activity
1. Give your learners a piece of blank paper,
and ask them to draw a picture of a house, a tree and a person.
2. After collecting the drawings, select three of the drawings
which are very different from each other. Write a paragraph
noting the ways in which the drawings differ.
3. Now compare what you have found with the findings of other
members of your group. |
The drawings you have collected will have some
similarities because the learners in your class group should be
within a year or two of each other. (If you have a wide age range
in your class, though, the differences you see may be much greater).
Piaget carefully watched his own children’s cognitive development,
and then worked on a series of tasks which were done with children
of various ages in order to look at the differences between them.
We will not look at the tasks at this point, but rather consider
the four basic stages identified by Piaget after many years of gathering
information about children’s development.
However, before we look at the developmental stages,
we need to make links with some of the terms we discussed in the
introduction to Piaget's theory. You will remember the terms ‘assimilation’,
‘accommodation’ and ‘equilibration’. These
refer to a feature of cognitive organisation which Piaget called
the ‘functions’. He believes
that these functions remain the same throughout the child’s
development – that these are the ways in which the human adapts
to the environment. You will also remember that we referred to the
term ‘schema’. These schemas form the basis of the cognitive
structures – and it is these that
change as the child matures.
Piaget believed that cognitive development was
an ongoing process of organisation and reorganisation of structures
and schemas. As long as we are faced with new situations and experiences
in our lives, we will need to continually organise and adapt our
existing schemas to make room for this new information. People are
involved in a life-long process of cognitive development. Obviously,
the content of thought does differ with age. A child has fewer and
more simple schemas in its early stages of life. As a child gets
older, s/he develops more and more schemas and these become ever
more expanded and complex.
Piaget noticed that in certain age ranges, children’s
thinking changes quite substantially, because of the reorganisation
and expanding of understanding that occurs in their natural development.
Piaget believed that the structure of a child’s schemas undergo
systematic change at particular points in development.
Piaget thus chose to break up the course of development
into four stages, which we will now explore. These stages reflect
the changes in the type of schemas available as a child develops
and the way these schemas are organised into cognitive structures.
According to Piaget, the rate at which an individual
progresses through these stages is variable, but the sequence of
the stages is the same for all children.
Piaget
Piaget recognised that learners don’t all
learn at the same rate or the same pace.
What implications does this belief
have for teachers in the classroom?
All learning and teaching, therefore, needs
to be flexible and learner-centred. This means that the learners
dictate the pace and rate of their own learning. Teachers, then,
need to provide learners with the appropriate (suitable) time and
the appropriate (suitable) support (dictated to by the individual
needs of the learner), so that the learner’s individual potential
can be fulfilled at their own pace.
1. The Sensorimotor Stage (0 – 2years)
From the title of this stage – the sensorimotor
stage – we can work out that this stage in a child’s
development involves two important developmental processes at this
time:
- the development of the five senses
(sight, hearing, touch, taste and smell),
- and motor development (the child
learns to sit, crawl, walk and then run).
The child begins to form simple schemas involving
his/her senses and exploratory movement. You will have seen babies
exploring the world by touching objects, smelling them and trying
to put them into their mouths. This is their way of making sense
of their world at this stage of cognitive development, and should
therefore be encouraged.
Characteristics of this stage include:
- The child’s early random actions
change to become more organised activities in this time, as the
child shifts from total dependence on caregivers to becoming gradually
more independent.
- The child learns to see the self as separate
from others.
- The child seeks stimulation and likes
to touch and explore objects. The child learns about its physical
environment by using the senses.
- The child learns about objects as constant
even though the objects may move places, for example, realising
that a toy still exists even when it is out of sight behind a
piece of furniture.
- The infant’s world is described
as consisting only of his/her own actions, with objects only being
seen as a part of his/her motor schema’s.
2. The preoperational stage (2 –
7 years)
Characteristics of this stage include:
- The development of language is the crucial
change which leads to development in this stage. Children start
talking from about two years onward (although they might start
to use words from a bit earlier). The child’s experience
and exposure to others is crucial to his development.
- The child’s construction of knowledge
about the world is assisted by the development of symbols (which
include gestures, words, drawings and signs).
- The child’s world becomes more
stable as the child realises that things are more permanent.
- The child starts to deal with objects
and events using a memory which becomes rapidly more complex.
- The child is still quite self-centred.
A child at this stage still believes that everyone else sees things
in exactly the same way that s/he does. The child is unable to
see another person’s point of view, or even to see the physical
world from a different perspective.
- S/he can start to group objects on the
basis of one feature – like things which are yellow, or
metal; s/he learns to see similarities and differences.
- The child gradually begins to be able
to work with ordering things in sequence from smallest to biggest,
and begins to work with the idea of number.
If you were to fill two identical drinking glasses to the same level
with cold drink and place these in front of a child who was in the
pre-operational stage of development, and ask the child whether
there is a different amount of cold drink in each glass, s/he would
probably respond that there was the same amount of cold drink in
both glasses.
If you were then to carefully pour the contents
of one of these glasses into a taller, thinner glass, while the
child is watching, and then ask him/her which of the glasses now
contains more cold drink, s/he would most likely respond that the
taller, thinner glass had more cold drink in than the other.
This example illustrates that simple, visible changes can easily
mislead a child at the pre-operational stage into thinking that
much more fundamental changes have occurred. As the cold drink is
poured from the short, fat container into the tall, thin container,
the child believes that there is now a different volume of cold
drink.

Similarly, we would find that a pre-operational
child presented with the situation illustrated above, would believe
that there is a different quantity of sweets in the two rows. The
child would think that the longer, more widely spaced row of sweets
has more sweets than the shorter more closely spaced row. This perception
would occur even if the child could see that the same sweets were
being moved from one grouping to the other.
3. The concrete operations stage (7-12
years)
Characteristics of this stage include:
- They become more and more able to think
logically, step by step – hence they can perform a sequence
of operations.
- Information is managed if it is concrete
– that is, based on real objects and events which the child
experiences.
- Children learn to manipulate figures
and symbols (which are representations of reality), and begin
to solve problems in a more orderly way (though they may use trial
and error for a while).
- They realise that certain changes can
be made without altering everything, and that these changes can
be mentally reversed to the original condition. A child at this
stage of cognitive development would not be mislead by the glasses
of cold drink or the rows of sweets in the previous illustrations.
- At this stage they develop the ability
to classify. They are able to group objects according to a number
of characteristics.
- The child develops a better understanding
of time and space.
- The child has now developed a logical
way of thinking. However, this is still limited compared with
adult thinking.
4. The formal operations stage (13 years
onward)
Characteristics of this stage include:
- At this stage a person is able to make
generalisations about the world, and is able to engage in mental
trial and error before taking any physical actions.
- This is the stage where the learner
becomes more and more able to work with ideas and abstract thinking.
(Adolescents enjoy thinking about ‘what if …?’
questions, and playing with hypotheses.) The focus shifts from
what is, to what may be, where situations don’t
have to be actually
experienced for them to be imagined.
- In the concrete operations stage, the
child always starts with experience, while formal operations thinking
begins with possibilities and then checks these against reality.
Clearly, many of life’s problems and many of the tasks demanded
in further and higher education, cannot be carried out when a
learner’s thinking is limited to concrete operations.
- It is important to realise that these
do not replace the already developed concrete operations, but
become available in addition to them.
- It is important to realise that learners
move into this stage gradually, and it may only be in their late
teenage years that learners are fully able to think in this way.
Self-Activity
Construct a mind-map of Piaget’s theory. |
We do not study theorists because we believe that
their theories and ideas are ‘gospel’, to be followed
unquestioning, rigidly and absolutely. Remember that theories act
as guides. We need to look carefully and critically at them and
see them as merely ways in which we could better understand our
learners, and as offering suggestions for our teaching practices.
We must be prepared to use Piaget’s theory of stages flexibly,
and not expect sudden, clear-cut changes in cognition to occur at
predetermined ages.
|